CTS21
2
17
The Controversy Over Domestic Violence by Women
A Methodological, Theoretical, and Sociology of Science Anafysis
Murray A. Straus
OVERVIEW
The methodological part of this chapter analyzes the discrepancy between the more than 100 "family conflict" studies of domestic physical assaults (those using the Conflict Tactic Scales and similar approaches), and what can be called "crime studies" (i.e., the National Crime Victimization Survey and studies using police call data).
Family conflict studies, without exception, show about equal rates of assault by men and women. Crime studies, without exception, show much higher rates of assault by men, often 90% by men. Crime studies also find a prevalence rate (for both men and women) that is a small fraction of the rate
AUTHOR'S NOTE: Amearlier version of this chapter was presented at the Qaremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology on Violence in Intimate Relationships, Claremont Graduate University, Claremont, CA 28 February, 1998. I would like to thank the 1997-98 Family Research Laboratory seminar, Patricia Tjaden, and Kersti Yllo for many comments, criticisms and suggestions which greatly aided in revising the chapter. Their assistance does not necessarily imply endorsement of the views expressed. The research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant T32MH15161 and the University of New Hampshire. This is a publication of the Family Violence Research Program of the Family Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire. A program description and bibliography will be sent on request.
17
18
of assaults found by family conflict studies.
The difference in prevalence rates and in gender differences between the two types of studies probably occur because crime studies deal with only the small part of all domestic assaults that the participants experience as a crime, such as assaults which result in an injury serious enough to need medical attention, or assaults by a former partner.
These occur relatively rarely and tend to be assaults by men.
The theoretical part of the chapter seeks to provide an explanation for the discrepancy between the low rates of assault by women outside the family and the very high rates of assault by women within the family.
The sociology of science part of the chapter seeks to explain why the controversy over domestic assaults by women persists and is likely to continue. I argue that neither side can give up their-position because it would be tantamount to giving up deeply held moral commitments and professional roles. I conclude that society needs both perspectives. Neither side should give up their perspective. Rather they should recognize the circumstances to which each applies.
In"trie mid-1970s*my colleagues and I made the disturbing discovery that women physically assaulted partners in marital, cohabiting, and dating relationships as often as men assaulted their partners (Steinmetz, 1978; Straus, 1997; Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). This finding caused me and my former colleague, Suzanne Steinmetz, to be excommunicated as feminists. Neither of us has accepted that sentence, but it remains in force. So when Salman Rushdie was condemned to death for his heresy, we may have fell even more empathy than most people because we had also experienced many threats, including a bomb threat.
The vitriolic 20-year controversy (Straus, 1990c; Straus, 1992b; Straus, 1997) had largely subsided by 1997. There are a number of reasons the controversy subsided. One reason is the overwhelming accumulation of evidence from more than a hundred studies showing approximately equal assault rates.
Another is the explosive growth of marital and family therapy from a family systems perspective which assumes mutual effects. In addition, research by clinical psychologists such as O'Leary (O'Leary et al., 1989) brought psychologists face to face with the assaults by both parties, as compared to studies of clients of shelters for battered women.
In November 1997, however, the controversy was suddenly reignited by newspaper headlines declaring "Partners Unequal in Abuse" (Peterson, 1997). These headlines were based on findings from the "National Violence Against Women in America Survey" (called the NVAW survey from here on). The NVAW surveyed 8,000 women and 8,000 men representing 16,000 households. The study was sponsored by the National Institute of Justice and the Centers for Disease Control (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1997). The NVAW study found that men physically assaulted their female partners at three times the rate at which women engaged in such behavior.
19
Family Conflict Studies
The NVAW findings contradict findings from many studies of violence between dating, cohabiting, and married couples, including national household surveys in the U.S. and other countries. These studies, such as the National Family Violence Surveys (Straus & Gelles, 1990), the National Survey of Families and Households, and British and Canadian national surveys (Carrado, George, Loxam, Jones, & Templar, 1996; Grandin & Lupri, 1997) are presented to respondents as research on family problems. Following (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1997) I will refer to them as "family conflict" studies. 1 started to compile a table summarizing all the family conflict studies that reported assault rates for both men and women partners. But I stopped after tabulating 39 studies out of about 120 in my files because at that point I received a bibliography of 70 studies, all of which found that ". . . women are as physically aggressive, or more aggressive, than men in their relationships. .. . The aggregate sample size in the reviewed studies exceeds 58,000." (Fiebert, 1997, pp. 273).
1 then tallied the 39 studies in my own table and found that more than two thirds reported somewhat higher rates for assaults by women. Moreover, every one of the remainder found very high rates for women, usually rates as high or almost as high as the male assault rate. Whenever the study provided separate figures for "severe" assaults such as kicking and punching (as compared to "minor" assaults such as slapping and throwing things at a partner) the pattern of similarly high rates was also reported for severe assaults. In addition there is the tabulation of 21 studies of dating couples by Sugarman and Hotahng (1989) which led them to conclude "A . . . surprising finding ... is the higher proportion of females than males who self-report having expressed violence in a dating relationship."
Crime Studies
Despite the evidence from more than 100 of these family conflict and dating violence studies, the finding from the recent NVAW study of three assaults by men to one by women cannot be ignored because it is based on a well-conducted survey and the study was sponsored by respected agencies. Moreover, two other major sources of data show an even greater predominance by men. In fact they indicate that assaulting a partner is almost exclusively a crime committed by men. I will call these "crime studies." I grouped the following four studies or types of studies under this heading because they have in qommon that they are presented to respondents as studies of crime, crime victimization, personal safety, injury; or violence, rather than as studies of family problems and conflicts.
20
- National Crime Survey (NCS)
- National Crime Victimization Study (NCVS). This is a revision of the NCS
- Police statistics studies
- National Violence Against Women in America (NVAW) study. Because of the importance of the NVAW study a detailed analysis is available in a supplemental paper (Straus, 1998).
Given the enormous discrepancies between over 100 family conflict studies and the fewer but excellent crime studies, the first objective of this chapter is to identify the source of these discrepancies. I will present evidence which suggests that the discrepancies reflect differences in the methodology of crime studies as compared to family conflict studies.
The second objective of the chapter is theoretical. If it is true that, in their domestic relationships, women are as assaultive as men, that needs to be explained because it is inconsistent with cultural norms and beliefs which hold that women are much less violent than men, and inconsistent with data showing that in nonfamily situations, the rate of assault by women is only a fraction of the male assault rate. In short, the theoretical issue is how to explain the high rate of domestic assaults by women,
The third objective of the chapter is to explain why the controversy has persisted despite the evidence, and in my opinion is likely to continue. The most fundamental reason is that the controversy is rooted in deep-seated differences in the underlying moral agenda and professional roles of the two sides.
WHAT IS VIOLENCE?
Much of the controversy over "violence" by women occurs because the participants in the debate use the same word for different phenomena.
At one extreme are those who use violence as a synonym for any unjust or cruel state of affairs or maltreatment of another human being. Thus, marketing of baby formulas to mothers in underdeveloped countries is sometimes described as violence against children because of the harm it causes.
At the other extreme, are those who restrict violence to physical assaults, i.e., to acts carried out with he intention of causing another person physical pain or injury (Gelles & Straus, 1979), regardless of whether an injury actually occurs. The concluding section of the chapter will explain why "violence" is used so differently.
My own research has been carried out from the perspective that defines violence exclusively as a physical assault. This perspective recognizes the importance of injury, and also that physical assaults are not necessarily the most damaging type of maltreatment. For example, one can hurt deeply—
21
even drive someone to suicide—without lifting a finger. Verbal aggression may be even more damaging than being the victim of physical attacks (Straus & Sweet, 1992; Vissing,:Straus, Gelles, & Harrop, 1993).
Those like me, who focus on the act of assault, also recognize that women, on average, suffer much more frequent and more severe injury (physical, economic, and psychological) than men (Stets & Straus, 1990; Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980; Zlotnick, Kohn, Peterson, & Pearlstein, 1998). Consequently, it is necessary to explain the reasons for focusing exclusively on physical assaults. The immediate reason is that, with rare exception, the controversy has been about equal rates of physical assaults, not about whether women experience more injury. The more important reasons have to do with legal, social policy, and ethical considerations.
From a legal perspective, it is important to realize that injury is not required for the crime of assault. The National Crime Panel Report defines assault as "an unlawful physical attack by one person upon another" (U.S. Department of Justiceji976). Neither this definition, nor the definition used for reporting assaults to the FBI requires injury or bodily contact (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 1995). Thus, if a person is chased by someone attempting to hit them with a stick or stab them and they escape, the attack is still a felony level crime—an "aggravated assault"—even though they were not touched. However, it is also true that police, prosecutors, and juries are strongly influenced by whether the assault did result in an injury and the seriousness of the injury:
From a social policy perspective, despite the much lower probability of physical injury resulting from attacks by women, one of the main reasons why "minor" assaults by women are such an important problem is that they put women in danger of much more severe retaliation by men (Feld & Straus, 1989). Assaults by women also help perpetuate the now implicit, but once explicit cultural norms I that gave husbands the legal right to "physically chastise an errant wife''!(Calvert, 1974). The legacy of that norm continues to make the marriage license a hitting license for both parties. To end "wife beating," it is essential ifbr women to cease what may seem to be "harmless" slapping, kicking, or throwing things at a male partner who persists in some outrageous behavior orj'twon't listen to reason."
Assaults by womenralso need to be a focus of social policy because of the harm to children from growing up in a violent household. The link between partner violence and child behavior problems occurs not only when both partners are violent (about half of families with partner assaults), but also when the assaults are committed exclusively by the male partner (about a quarter of the cases), as well as when the assaults are committed exclusively by the female partner (Straus, 1992a).
The most fundamental reason for giving attention to assaults per se, regardless of whether!an injury occurs, is the intrinsic moral wrong of assaulting a partner. Assaults by women are a crime and a serious social
22
problem, just as it would be if men "only" slapped their wives or "only" slapped a fellow employee and produced no injury. Although this is a fundamental reason for morally condemning women who "only" slap their partners, it should not be allowed to obscure the fact that assaults by men are likely to be even more morally reprehensible because they result in injury so much more often than assaults by women. Nevertheless, an even greater wrong does not excuse the lesser wrong. A society in which dating, cohabiting, and married partners never hit each other is not a more unrealistic goal than a society in which coworkers never hit each other, and is certainly no less a hallmark of a humane society.
METHODOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS FOR THE DISCREPANT FINDINGS OF FAMILY CONFLICT AND CRIME STUDIES
Explaining Discrepancies in the Rate of Partner Assault
Before examining the differences between family conflict and crime studies that could explain the discrepancies in their findings on assaults by men and women, it is necessary to examine and explain the much lower rate of assault by both men and women found by crime studies. This is necessary because the same processes result in both the low prevalence rate for both men and women and the high ratio of male to female offenders.
The first row of Table 2.1 summarizes findings from many studies showing that, relative to family conflict studies, crime studies uncover a much lower number of domestic assaults. The rates from crime studies range from a low of 0.02% to 1.1%, whereas the rates from family conflict studies tend to be about 16%. The second row of Table 2.1 shows that disclosure of domestic assaults in crime studies is only a small fraction of the rate obtained by family conflict studies. Even with the improvement in the National Crime Survey to deal with the extremely low prevalence, the rate obtained by the revision is still only one eighteenth of the rate obtained by family conflict surveys (Bachman & Saltzman, 1995). Similarly, the recent NVAW study found only one fifteenth of the number of assaults that have been found by family conflict studies.
The low rate of assaults by both husbands and wives found by crime studies, and especially the NCS probably results from a number of situational and unintended "demand characteristics" (Orne, 1962) of the study. These surveys were presented as a study of crime, or in the case of the NVAW study, "personal safety," injury, and "violence " Unfortunately, when a survey of crime, violence, or injury is the context for estimating rates of domestic assault, the contextual message can take precedence over specific instructions to include all assaults, regardless of the perpetrator and regardless of whether injury resulted. The contextual message can lead some resoondents to mis-
Domestic Violence by Women
TABLE 2.1. Variations in Domestic Assault Statistics
23
National
Family National Crime Police
Conflict Crime Victim Call NVAW
Studies Survey Survey Data Study1
Annual Assault Rate 16% 0.2% 0.9% 0.2%3 1.1%
Fraction of Family ------;--------> l/80th 1/lSth L/SOth 1/15th
Conflict Rate
Injury Rate 1.3% 75% 52% unknown 76%
Male Rate (top) & 112% 0.4% 0.76% 90% male 5 1.3
Female Rate
12.4%4 0.03% 0.11% 0.9
Male to Female Ratio i 1:1 13:1 7:1 9:1 1.4:1
1. National Violence Against Women Survey (Tjaden & Thoennes, 1998).
2. The lower end of this range is for married couples. The upper end reflects the large number of studies which show that rate for dating couples of 25% to 40% (Sugarman & Hotaling, 1989). Stets and Straus (1990) attribute the high rate to the youthfulness of dating couples. They found that, for young married couples, the rates were even higher than for dating couples of that age. ;:
3. Based on all aggravated assaults known to the police in 1994 (Federal Bureau of Investigation, 1995) because Uniform Crime Reports (UCR) do not distinguish between domestic assaults and other assaults. In 1994 the UCR rate of assaults 430 per hundred thousand population (0.4%). An unknown fraction of these were domestic assaults. To be on the safe side, I used half of that as domestic assaults.
4. To avoid bias due to possible under-reporting by men of their own assaults, these rates were computed from information provided by the 2,994 women in the 1985 National Family Violence Survey. The rates given are for any assault. The ratio was essentially the same for minor and severe assaults. See Straus, J 994,
5. The rates for men and women cannot be given because police call data are only reported as the percent of cases with a male offender, not as rates pet 100 or 1,000 men and women.
perceive the study as being concerned only with assaults that are experienced as a crime or as violence, or assaults that resulted in or are likely to result in injury . However, only a small percentage of domestic assaults are experienced as a crime or as -aj; threat to personal safety or violence. For example, while being slapped or kicked by a partner may be experienced as horrendous or despicable, it takes relatively rare circumstances to perceive it as a "crime" (Ferraro, 1989; Ferraro & Johnson, 1983; Langan & Innes, 1986). One such circumstance is an injury. Injury serious enough to need medical attention occurs in only 1% to 3% of domestic assaults on women and 0.5% of domestic assaults on men (Stets &. Straus, 1990; Zlotnick, Kohn, Peterson, &. Pearl-stein, 1998). To the extent that it takes injury for a respondent in a crime survey to perceive there |is something to report, the low injury rate is part of the explanation for the extremely low rate of partner assaults found by the crime studies. This does hot mean that all respondents misperceived what was
24
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
expected in this way. Indeed, 49% of NCVS domestic assault victims reported no injury, but this is still much less than the 97% to 99% reporting no injury in family conflict studies (see next paragraph).
The middle row of Table 2.1 gives the injury rates from family conflict and crime studies. It shows rates of 3% or less for family conflict studies, and rates of 75%, 52%, and 76% for the NCS, the NCVS, and the NVAW study. The implausibly high injury rates from the latter three studies are probably the result of the crime and threat to safety focus of those studies, i.e., partner assaults tend to not be reported unless there is something, such as injury, that moves them from the category of a "family fight" to a "crime'* or a threat to safety. So assaults resulting in injury make up an extremely large proportion of the cases that are reported.
Another similar process probably accounts for the extremely high rate of assaults by former partners in crime studies. That is, one of the circumstance leading a respondent in crime studies to report an assault by a partner is if the attack is by a former spouse. That makes it a "real crime" because a former spouse "has no right to do that." Even with the revisions intended to avoid this problem, the NCVS found 25 times more assaults by former partners in the previous 12-month period than by current partners (Bachman & Saltzman, 1995). Given the vastly greater time exposure to current spouses during the 12-month referent period, that ratio does not seem plausible.
In summary, assaults by a partner are most likely to be experienced as a crime if the attacks result in injury or if it is an attack by a former partner. To the extent that this is correct, it helps explain the drastically lower prevalence rate for intimate partner assaults in crime studies because those circumstances are relatively rare.
Explaining Discrepancies in the Ratio of Male to Female Offenders
The last two rows of Table 2.1 show the contrast between the roughly equal rates for family conflict studies and the predominance of men in the crime studies and the NVAW study. Although a number of factors are likely to be involved, the methodological differences described in the preceding section are an important part of the explanation. Specifically, the same demand characteristics of a crime study that produced the extremely low rates for both men and women, also produce the high ratio of male to female offenders. If one of the circumstances that leads a respondent to experience being hit by a partner as a crime, or one of the circumstances that leads a police officer to make an arrest, is an injury that needs medical attention, and if assaults resulting in injury are much more likely to occur when a man is the offender, it follows that men predominate in statistics based on crime surveys or crime reports. The following sections examine each of the sources of data from that perspective.
Domestic Violence by Womeii
25
National Crime Survey and National Crime Victimization Survey
In 1992, after 10 years of considering revisions to reduce demand characteristics such as those just discussed, the National Crime Survey (NCS) was revised to secure more complete reporting of crimes in which the offender is often an intimate, such as tape and assault, and renamed as the National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS). The result of these changes, as predicted, was to quadruple the overall prevalence rate for domestic assaults from 2.2 to 8.7 per thousand, i.e., from 0J2% to 0.9% (Bachman & Saltzman, 1995, Table 5). A change that was not predicted by the NCVS sponsors, but which follows from the demand characteristics explanation for the low prevalence rate in crime studies, was to decrease the ratio of male to female assaults on partners from 13:1 to 7:1. The injury rate also decreased (as predicted on the basis of the demand characteristics explanation) from 75% to 52%. Thus the redesign helped to reduce the demand characteristics that probably account for the extremely low prevalence rate, the implausibly high injury rate, and the high ratio of male to female-1 assaults. Nevertheless, the statistics just presented suggests that the demand characteristic problem persists to a significant degree.
Another consideration that may lead to under reporting of assaults on partners by women as a crime or a threat to safety is that such attacks are often discounted as a jok£ because, for men, the risk of injury and therefore fear of injury, is relatively low. In one of my early interviews on domestic assaults, I asked the husband if his wife had ever hit him. He stood up and shoved his shoulder toward me as if I were his wife. He said "Yeah, I told her, go ahead and hit me." Fpf still another group, the idea of being assaulted by one's wife may be so threatening to their masculine identity that they would be ashamed to report it id the NCVS interviewer.
i !
Police Statistics
Statistics based oil:analysis of police calls for domestic disturbances result in a rate of assaults by men that is hugely greater than the rate of assaults by women. Dobajsh and Dobash (1979), for example, found that 99% of intrafamily assault cases in two Scottish cities were assaults by men. Since then, a number of studiesbf statistics based on police calls show that men are eight or nine times more often the assailants than women. However, the proportion of men maybe declining slightly. To take one recent example, Brookhoff (1997) foundithat 78% of assailants in police calls were men. This is less than 90 or 99%, biit it is still extremely high.
The proportion of incidents of partner assault known to the police, like the proportion in the NtfVS, is only a small faction of the number of such incidents estimated on the basis of family conflict studies. Kaufman Kantor
26
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
and Straus (1990) for example, examined a nationally representative sample of domestic assaults and found that 93% were not reported to the police, Given the absence of such a large proportion of cases from police statistics, an understanding of what might be filtering out most of the cases can provide a clue to gender differences in police statistics on partner assault.
The high percentage of male offenders in police statistics reflects the circumstances that lead to a police call. An obvious circumstance is that a call to the police for help is much more likely if there is injury or imminent danger of injury. Because injury is more likely if the assailant is a male, assaults by men are much more likely than assaults by women to occasion a police call. If the injury rate for assaults by men is about seven times greater than the injury rate for assaults by women (Stets & Straus, 1990), and if injury is a virtually a requirement for the police to be called in, that alone would contribute importantly to producing a 7:1 ratio. It is also one of the main reasons why I have always insisted that ". . . although women may assault their partners at approximately the same rate as men, because of the greater physical, financial, and emotional injury suffered by women, they are the predominant victims. Consequently, first priority in services for victims and in prevention and control must continue to be directed toward assaults by husbands" (Straus, 1997).
Other reasons assaults by women are rare in police statistics probably include the reluctance of men to admit that they cannot "handle their wives." In addition, although police in some jurisdictions are now arresting female offenders more than previously, analogous to their former reluctance to make arrests of husbands, they remain reluctant to make arrests in such cases (Cook, 1997). Still another factor that probably influences the probability of police involvement is drunken, loud, and destructive behavior, and those are more often male than female accompaniments of partner assault. This is especially likely to be the case among the low-income and low-education families where partner assaults and police calls for partner assaults are most common.
NVAW Study
1 classified the National Violence Against Women (NVAW) study as a crime study because it was presented to respondents as a study of "personal safety" and that term is used repeatedly. In addition, the second question in the survey was "Do you think violent crime is more or less of a problem for men today than previously." This question, at the very start of this study, can signal to respondents that the study is about crime. The tone of the NVAW keeps threats, injuries, violence, and safety constantly before the respondent. In this context, reporting that one's partner has done any of the things asked about is the same as saying that the partner is a criminal or is about to injure them.
26
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
and Straus (1990) for example, examined a nationally representative sample of domestic assaults and found that 93% were not reported to the police. Given the absence of such a large proportion of cases from police statistics, an understanding of what might be filtering out most of the cases can provide a clue to gender differences in police statistics on partner assault,
The high percentage of male offenders in police statistics reflects the circumstances that lead to a police call. An obvious circumstance is that a call to the police for help is much more likely if there is injury or imminent danger of injury. Because injury is more likely if the assailant is a male, assaults by men are much more likely than assaults by women to occasion a police call. If the injury rate for assaults by men is about seven times greater than the injury rate for assaults by women (Stets & Straus, 1990), and if injury is a virtually a requirement for the police to be called in, that alone would contribute importantly to producing a 7:1 ratio. It is also one of the main reasons why I have always insisted that ". . . although women may assault their partners at approximately the same rate as men, because of the greater physical, financial, and emotional injury suffered by women, they are the predominant victims. Consequently, first priority in services for victims and in prevention and control must continue to be directed toward assaults by husbands" (Straus, 1997).
Other reasons assaults by women are rare in police statistics probably include the reluctance of men to admit that they cannot "handle their wives." In addition, although police in some jurisdictions are now arresting female offenders more than previously, analogous to their former reluctance to make arrests of husbands, they remain reluctant to make arrests in such cases (Cook, 1997). Still another factor that probably influences the probability of police involvement is drunken, loud, and destructive behavior, and those are more often male than female accompaniments of partner assault. This is especially likely to be the case among the low-income and low-education families where partner assaults and police calls for partner assaults are most common.
NVAW Study
I classified the National Violence Against Women (NVAW) study as a crime study because it was presented to respondents as a study of "persona! safety" and that term is used repeatedly. In addition, the second question in the survey was "Do you think violent crime is more or less of a problem for men today than previously." This question, at the very start of this study, can signal to respondents that the study is about crime. The tone of the NVAW keeps threats, injuries, violence, and safety constantly before the respondent. In this context, reporting that one's partner has done any of the things asked about is the same as saying that the partner is a criminal or is about to injure them.
Domestic Violence by Women
27
These and other aspects of the wording and questions, which are detailed in a supplemental paper (Straus, 1998), may have created a set of demand characteristics that led many respondents to perceive the NVAW as a study of injury and crime, and therefore to restrict their reports to assaults that have resulted in injury or ard experienced as a threat to safety or as a "crimes." To the extent that this occurred, it would exclude most instances of assault by a partner, and especially the culturally acceptable "harmless" assaults by women, and result in the 3:1 ratio of male to female offenders featured in the press release on the NVAW study. If this explanation is correct, the NVAW study does not contradict the large number of family conflict studies which show that women initiate and carry out assaults on male partners at about the same rate as men attack^ female partners.
A puzzling aspect iof the NVAW study needs to be explained. The 3:1 ratio featured in the [press release is based on lifetime prevalence rates (assaults by any partner^ past or present). However, to be comparable to other studies, the published report (Tjaden and Thoennes, 1998) also had to present annual prevalence rates (assaults in the past year). These rates are given in the last column of Table 2.1 in this chapter. They show a 1.4:1 ratio of male to female offenders, which is much closer to the 1:1 ratio of family conflict studies. One possible explanation for the 3:1 lifetime rate goes back to the threat to safety context of the NVAW study. If one assumes that events experienced as a threat to safety are more likely to be remembered over a long time period, and that women are more likely to experience a slap or a kick by their partner as a threiat to safety, it follows that women will remember and be able to report a larger percent of the assaults that occurred years back than men will be able to remember.
Family Conflict Studies
i
The near equal rates of assault found by family conflict studies have been attributed to a different set of methodological problems. These include purported defects in the iGIonflict Tactics Scales or CTS (Straus, 1979; Straus, 1990a; Straus, HambyjBoney-McCoy, &. Sugarman, 1996), under reporting of assaults by male; respondents, and failure to take into account self-defensive violence by women and injury.
Under Reporting by Men
Family conflict stiidies often interview one partner to find out about the relationship, i.e., whaitithe respondent has done and also what the partner has done. When men are trie respondents, they may minimize their own violence and exaggerate violence by their partner. However, that cannot be the explanation for the equal rates because, regardless of whether the information
28
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
comes from a male or female respondent, family conflict studies have found about equal rates of assault by the male and female partner (Straus, 1997).
The Conflict Tactics Scales
Most of the family conflict studies used the Conflict Tactics Scales or CTS, and the near equality in rates of assault by men and women has been attributed to purported biases in the CTS (Straus, 1990a; Straus, 1997, for a discussion of the purported biases). Consequently, it is important to examine studies that used other methods. An early study by Scanzoni (1978), asked a sample of 321 women what they did in cases of persistent conflict with their husband. Sixteen percent reported trying to hit the husband. Sorenson and Telles's (1991) analysis of 2,392 households in the Los Angeles Epidemiological Catchment Area Study found that "women reported higher rates .. .(than men)." The National Survey of Families and Households asked "During the past year, how many fights with your partner resulted in (you/ him/her) hitting, shoving, or throwing things at (you/him/her)." Zlotnick (1998), analyzed this data for the 5,474 couples in the sample and found very similar rates for assaults by men and women (5% rate of assaults by men and 4% of assaults by women).
Self Defense and Injury
In my early research on domestic assaults, it seemed so obvious that women were injured more often and more seriously than men, and that domestic assaults by women were primarily in self defense, that I did not collect data on injury and self defense. I simply asserted it as a self-evident fact (Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). So, when, in the 1985 National Family Violence Survey, I did ask who was the first to hit, I was surprised to find that half of the women respondents reported they had hit first (Stets & Straus, 1990). Several other studies (Bland & Orn, 1986; Carrado, George, Loxam, Jones, & Templar, 1996; Demaris, 1992; Gryl, Stith, & Bird, 1991; Sorenson & Telles, 1991) also found about equal rates of initiation by men and women.
Family conflict studies rarely measure who is injured. The original CTS did not obtain data on injury. The CTS2 (Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996) includes a supplemental scale to measure injury, but retains the system of measuring assaults regardless of injury. However, when injury data has been obtained along with the CTS and this is used as a criterion for estimating "violence," the rate drops to a one that is similar to the extremely low rate found in crime studies, and the percentage of assaults by men also increases to approximately the ratio found in crime studies (Straus, 1991; Straus, 1997). These findings further support the idea that one of the main reasons crime studies find that domestic assaults are overwhelmingly com-
Domestic Violence by Women
29
mitted by men is because they tend to omit so many of the assaults that do not result in injury.
APPROPRIATE USES OF FAMILY CONFLICT AND CRIMINAL JUSTIQE OR INJURY DATA
The explanations just idffered for the discrepancies between family conflict studies and crime studies or injury studies suggests that the findings from the two types of studies apply to different groups of people and reflect different aspects of domestic assault. Most of the violence that is revealed by surveys of family problems is relatively minor and relatively infrequent, whereas a large portion of the violence in crime studies and clinical studies is chronic and severe and often involves injuries that need medical attention. These two types of violence probably have ^different etiology and probably require a different type of intervention (see Johnson, 1995). If this is correct, it is important to avoid using findings based on eases known to police or shelters for battered women as the basis for understanding and dealing with the relatively minor and less chronic violence found in the population in general. That type of unwarranted generalization is often made and it is known as the "clinical fallacy."
Family conflict studies pose the opposite problem. It can be called the "representative sample fallacy" (Straus, 1990b). Family conflict studies contain very few cases involving weekly or daily assaults and injury. Consequently, family conflict studies may provide an erroneous basis for policies and interventions focuked on these relatively few but extreme cases. This is a serious shortcoming because although the numbers may be relatively low, they are the cases that pose the most serious problems and which need to have priority in respect to interventions.
Much of the controversy over assaults by women stems from assuming that data from family conflict studies on assaults by women apply to cases known to the police andjshelters, and the similar unwarranted assumption that the predominance of assaults by men in data from crime studies and battered women shelters appliesfto the population at large.
Both family conflict study data and also data from crime and battered women shelter samples are needed. Findings based on crime studies are needed to provide information on the more serious types of domestic assaults and therefore to provide a realistic basis for programs designed to aid the victims and to end this type of domestic violence. At the same time, to understand assaults onl partners in the general population, which seldom involve injury, and for informing "primary prevention" efforts (Cowen, 1978), it is crucial to have family conflict data, including data on the most minor and "harmless" slap. In fact, understanding assaults that respondents do not think of as "a crime" may be the most important information for purposes of primary prevention because that is where more serious assaults start.
30
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
A SOCIOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE ON ASSAULTS BY WOMEN WITHIN AND OUTSIDE THE FAMILY
One reason the repeated findings on equal rates of partner assault by men and women have been suspect is the absence of a theory to explain those findings. A similar doubt and incredulity existed when the extremely high rate of partner assaults by men was first brought to public and scientific attention by feminists. This was soon followed by a rich theoretical analysis from both a feminist and other perspectives (Gelles & Straus, 1979; Straus, 1976).
Although that sort of theoretical development has not yet happened for domestic assaults by women, some of the principles explaining domestic assaults by men also apply to women; for example, the proposition from criminology and exchange theory that the prevalence of crime is partly a function of what one can get away with. Since domestic assaults usually occur in private, both men and women can get away with it and this contributes to the high rate of domestic assault for both genders. However, the unique circumstances explaining assaults by women have not been analyzed theoretically. Table 2.2 makes a start in developing such a theory. It identifies variables that are hypothesized to inhibit physical assaults by women outside the family and variables that are hypothesized to facilitate physical assaults within the family by women. I label it a sociological perspective because, not surprisingly, as a sociologist, most of the processes I identify refer to social norms, social relationships, and social control processes. It is also important to keep in mind that many of the starting propositions are in the form of assumptions rather than empirically demonstrated propositions. So what follows is intended as a basis for further theoretical development and empirical research.
Different Cultural Norms For Women and Men
Violence Is Rule Following Behavior
Row A of Table 2.2 is based on the assumption that most human behavior, including acts of violence, is influenced by cultural norms. Typically, the actors are unaware they are following cultural prescriptions. I have often asked students in my introductory sociology class to do small experiments based on the work of Garfinkel (1964), to demonstrate behavior guided by unperceived cultural rules. For example, I asked them, the next time they go home, to be very polite and always say please and thank you. Their notes on what happened almost invariably reveal expressions of concern such as "Are you OK. Are you sure everything is OK?" This exercise brings to light an implicit cultural rule which allows and expects a relaxation within the family of the usual standards of civil social interaction,
Interpersonal violence, both within the family and outside the family also tends to follow unperceived rules and cultural scripts (Black, 1983; Kennedy
Domestic Violence by Women
31
TABLE 2.2. What Explains Equal Assault Rates By Women in the Family, But Much Lower Rates^Elsewhere?
Inhibitors of Assault By Women Outside the Family
Facilitators of Assault By Women Within the Family
A. Cultural Norms: "Unfeminine" for women to hit, but "manly" for men.
A. Cultural Norms: An indignant woman slapping a man's face epitomizes femininity to many
• "if he gets fresh, slap him"
• survey data "ok for a wife to slap"
• examples in media
B. Lesser Size and Strength: Makes women fearful of retaliation and injury hy someone who is not committed to them.
B. Lesser Size and Strength: "I knew I wouldn't hurt him."
C. Self Defense or Retaliation: Low because women assaulted less often (except for rape).
C. Self Defense or Retaliation: High because women assaulted frequently by
partners.
D. Gender Norms for Conflict: Outside the family, women interact more with women and men more with men and male culture is more pro violence as a means of conflict resolution.
D. Gender Norms for Conflict: In couple relations, male partners may be less reachable with non-violent problem solving that works in woman-to-woman relationships. This increases probability of violence to force attention to the problem.
E. Source of Identity: Women's identity is not as strongly basedion extra-family interests. Therefore less need to defend interests and reputationjby violence.
E. Source of Identity: Women's identity is as strongly or more strongly based on family than men's. Therefore equal need o defend interests and reputation,
F. Violence Level of Setting: Women are less often in high violence occupations: those requiring violence (police, military, some sports) and jobs with high violence rates such as heavy physical laborjobs.
Violence Level of Setting: Women spend more time at home, and 90% hit toddlers. Mothers get 5 to 14 years of practice in hitting as morally correct through corporal punishment of their own children.
G. Criminal Justice System Involvement: Police involvement notjgreatly different for men & women.
G. Criminal Justice System Involvement:
• Men not likely to call the police.
• Police are not likely to arrest women, so women can get away with it even more than men.
32
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
& Sacco, 1998; Luckenbill, 1977; Zimring & Hawkins, 1997). For example, lashing out physically when infidelity is discovered, is typically expenenced as something that happens when a person "loses control." However, a large percentage of the population finds hitting an unfaithful partner to be "understandable." This indicates an implicit cultural norm that permits one to "lose it" when a partner is unfaithful, and even more so when the partner is flagrantly unfaithful. Thus, "losing control" under those circumstances is culturally expected (Ptacek, 1988).
An implicit norm that helps explain the high rate of domestic assaults by women makes the marriage licence a hitting licence (Straus & Hotaling, 1980). That implicit norm is a carry over from a previous explicit legal norm that held sway until the 1870s when the courts ceased to recognue the common law right of husbands to "physically chastise an erring wife," i.e., the right to use corporal punishment (Calvert, 1974). Other norms permitting violence include the legal right of parents to hit children for purposes of correction and control.
Gender Differences in Rules for Violence
There are also norms about violence that are different for men and women, and different for behavior inside and outside the family. These are illustrated in Row A of Table 2.2 As indicated in this row, physical assaults by women, if outside the family, are considered "unfeminine" but in the family and other intimate partner relationships, physical attacks by women are expected and lauded under certain circumstances. For example, in my generation, probably millions of young women were told by their mother, "If he gets fresh, slap him" rather than "If he gets fresh, leave immediately." Then as now, slapping a man who does or says something outrageous is often seen as quintessentially feminine. Kathleen Willie, who charged President Clinton with unwanted sexual touching, said that she should have slapped his face ". . . but I don't think you can slap the president of the United States." In short, because of his position of power, she failed to follow the culturally prescribed script. Other examples can be seen every day on television or in films.
Perhaps the most direct evidence of the norm permitting assault by women against male partners comes from the survey done for the National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence, appointed in the wake of the upsurge of violence in the 1960s (National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence, 1969). My colleagues and I replicated that study in 1985, 1992, and 1995 (Straus, Kaufman Kantor, & Moore, 1997). In 1968, more than one out of five American adults (22%) believed there are circumstances when it is permissible for a wife to slap her husband's face. That percentage has remained essentially the same up through our latest survey in 1995. The percentage approving a husband slapping his wife was slightly
Domestic Violence by Women
33
lower in 1968 (20%), but it declined to half that by 1995. So society is becoming less tolerant of assaults by husbands, but continues to find such assaults acceptable if done by a wife.
Size and Strength Differences
Row B of Table 2.2 suggests that the smaller average size and muscle development of women contributes to both the lower rate of assaults outside the family and the equal rate within the family. Outside the family this physical disadvantage tends to make women fearful of retaliation and injury by someone who is not committed to them, and hence to avoid physical confrontation. But inside the family, a combination of belief that the partner will not really hurt them, and the belief that hitting is okay because, as many women told me, "I knew I wouldn't hurt him" (in the sense of no physical injury as compared to causing pain) reduces inhibitions about hitting the partner and limits fear of retaliation. Statistical evidence consistent with this interpretation comes from a study by Fiebert and Gonzalez (1997). They found that 29% of their sample of 978 women college students reported having hit a male partner. Of the women who had hit, two thirds (62%) checked as one of the reasons "I do not believe my actions would hurt my partner" or "I believe that men can readily protect themselves so I don't worry when I become physically aggressive." The lower probability of injury for assaults by women is probably also one of the reasons why the cultural norms are more tolerant of assaults by women on their partners.
Self Defense or Retaliation
Row C of Table 2.2 is based on the fact that the risk of a woman being assaulted by a stranger (even after including the risk of being raped) is less than half of the risk to which men are exposed (Craven, 1996). But within the family, the risk of a woman being assaulted by a partner is very high. The implications of this difference stem from the fact that almost all assaults are attempts to correct what the assailant perceives as the misbehavior of the victim, such as "making a pass at my wife" or not paying back a loan, or an insulting remark. The probability of a woman getting into a physical fight with a man other than her partner who has attacked her to correct some perceived misbehavior is low. Hence the probability of a nonfamily assault is also low. On the other hand, the probability of being attacked by a male partner for some perceived misbehavior is high. Given the norms of American society which favor retaliation (as illustrated by a parent advising a child "If hit, hit back") the probability of a woman physically retaliating against her partner is therefore also high.
34
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
Gender Norms for Conflict
Row D of Table 2.2 is based on the assumption that women are more inclined to discuss disagreements and men are more inclined to use an avoidance strategy. Conversely, physical attacks are more acceptable as a means of conflict resolution among men than women. Consequently, the fact that, outside the family, women interact more with other women than with men means that they interact more with a less violence-prone part of the population, and this reduces the chances of a nonfamily conflict escalating into violence relative to the same conflict between two men. In couple relations, on the other hand, women interact with male partners who may be less reachable through discussion than in woman-to-woman relationships. When faced with a man who withdraws and refuses to talk about a problem, and the problem persists, as it often does in the family, many women resort to slapping, kicking, and throwing things in an attempt to coerce the partner to attend to the issue. Fiebert and Gonzales's (1997) data on 285 college women who had assaulted a partner are consistent with this line of reasoning. When asked about reasons for hitting the partner, 38% checked "I wished to gain my partner's attention" and 43% checked "My partner was not listening to me." Because there was likely to be some overlap of the two responses, these data can only indicate that somewhere between 43% (complete overlap) and 81% (no overlap) indicated violence to physically coerce the partner's attention.
Source of Identity
Row E in Table 2.2 is based on research (discussed earlier) indicating that most violent acts are intended to correct the misbehavior (as the assailant sees it) of the victim. One of the most important categories of misbehavior consists of slighting or denigrating personal identity. Assuming that the identity of women is less closely linked to the public world outside the family than is the identity of men; women, on average, have fewer occasions and less motivation for defending nonfamily interests and reputation by violence. On the other hand, interpersonal relationships are more important in the lives of women than men (Gilligan, 1982). The identity of women is as strongly or more strongly based on what occurs within the family than is the case for men. Consequently, the need for women to defend their interests and identity in family roles is at least as great as for men. Moreover, the probability of needing to do so is increased because of cultural norms which presume that the husband is the head of the household. This creates a situation in which male partners feel privileged to direct and evaluate the behavior of their partners in the very spheres of life that are so crucial for a woman's identity. Far more women than men, for example, are likely to be deeply offended and hurt by negative comments on their cooking, tastes in household furnishing, or methods of child care.
Domestic Violence by Women
35
Violence Level of Setting
The left column of Row F in Table 2.2 is based on the sex segregation of the labor market. The large differences in type of employment that are typical of men and women locate many men, but few women, in job settings where the assault rate is high. This includes occupations directly involving violence such as the and the military, and some sports. It also includes occupations requiring heavy physical labor, which perhaps because they are male occupations, tend to have higher rates of violence than do white collar occupations. Thus, women are not involved in the violence associated with many male occupational settings and this keeps their nonfamily assault rate down. The same principle applies to the lower involvement of women in many violence prone recreational settings, such as visiting bars (Felson, 1997).
The right column of row F refers to the domestic setting. Ironically, the domestic setting tends to be high in violence, even in the households of college educated couples (Gelles & Straus, 1988; Straus, Gelles, & Stein-metz, 1980). The violence in domestic settings that has come to public attention in recent decades (but has always been present) is assaults on spouses. But in addition, households are the locus of the most violent relationship in American families — the relation between parents and children. Over 90% of parents hit toddlers (Straus, 1994; Straus & Stewart, 1999) and over a third are still doing it when the children are in their early teens. Because mothers are the primary caretakers, more of this is done by women.
The violent child rearing that American children experience under such euphemisms as "physical punishment," "spanking" "a good swat on the behind," and "a good slap on the face for mouthing off," has many profound negative consequences which are only now starting to be recognized (Straus, 1994; Straus & Paschall, 1998; Straus, Sugarman, & Giles-Sims, 1997). One of these is that having been hit repeatedly by parents as a child (the typical experience of American children) and hitting one's own children provide role practice in violence to correct misbehavior, and this tends to spill over to correcting the misbehavior of the mother's partner (Straus, 1994; Straus & Yodanis, 1996, Chart 7-3), thus constituting one more factor contributing to the high rate of partner assault by women.
Criminal Justice System Involvement
Traditionally, police and prosecutors have been reluctant to become involved in the crime of assault, regardless of whether it occurs outside the family or inside (Buzawa & Buzawa, 1996). One of the reasons is that arrests and prosecutions for assault do not receive the public recognition of arresting, for example, a robber. Moreover, assaults are typically in the form of "fights" involving both parties and, legally, both parties should be prosecuted regardless of who started it. Under those circumstances, the probability of a case actually being tried, much less resulting in a conviction, are low relative to
36
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
arrests for other crimes. Such cases are regarded as "trouble." Police and prosecutors do not get much recognition, and may even be faulted, if there are very many such cases in their record. Outside the family, those principles apply to both men and women and the risk of police involvement is not greatly different for men and women.
As a result of the women's movement, the traditional reluctance of the police to become involved in "domestics" (Kaufman Kantor & Straus, 1990; Straus, 1977) has changed. In most jurisdictions in the United States, state laws or police regulations now require or recommend arrest. However, consistent with the greater injury rate for women, these laws and regulations may state or imply a male offender. Although on average, when there is an injury, this is correct, it denies male victims equal protection under the law. In fact, there are a growing number of complaints that attempts by men to obtain police protection may result in the man being arrested (Cook, 1997). That ironic situation is an additional reason that men are reluctant to call for police protection. The main reason is one already discussed in explaining gender differences in police statistics: The injury rate is much lower when the offender is a woman and there is therefore less perceived need to call for protection. The fact that assault is a legal and moral crime, regardless of whether there is injury, is lost from view.
Men are also less likely to call the police, even when there is injury, because, like women, they feel shame about disclosing family violence. But for many men, the shame is compounded by the shame of not being able to keep their wives under control. Among this group, a "real man" would be able to keep her under control. Moreover, many police share these same traditional gender role expectations. This adds to the legal and regulatory presumption that the offender is a man. As a result, the police are reluctant to arrest women for domestic assault. Women know this. That is, they know they are likely to be able to get away with it. As in the case of other crimes, the probability of a woman assaulting her partner is strongly influenced by what she thinks she can get away with (Gelles & Straus, 1988).
The Theoretical Agenda
To convert the above discussion of factors that inhibit and facilitate assaults by women in different settings into a theory requires expanding the number of etiological factors considered and less exclusive reliance on social factors. It requires tracing out the links and interactions among the factors. Although developing such a theory is a large task, even the limited discussion in this chapter may help advance research on gender differences in partner assaults. Such a theoretical development is needed because one of the paradoxical aspects of science is that although empirical data are the ultimate determinants of what is "scientific knowledge," empirical facts by themselves are suspect until there is a plausible theory to explain them. My hope is that
Domestic Violence by Women
37
by providing at least part of the needed theoretical understanding, this chapter may contribute to transforming the question of gender differences in partner assault from the realm of polemics and recrimination to the realm of theoretically based empirical research.
A SOCIOLOGY OF SCIENCE ANALYSIS OF THE CONTROVERSY
The previous paragraph assumes that the parties involved would like to see the controversy over domestic assaults by women transformed from the realm of polemics to the realm of theoretically based empirical research. But that is not necessarily the case. In order to understand why it may not be the case, it is helpful to return to the question how violence is defined. This is because much of the controversy over violence by women occurs as a result of each side using "violence" to refer to something different. I will call these two approaches broad and narrow definitions. The focus on broad versus narrow conceptions of violence is a huresitc device to help identify some of the underlying issues. Moreover, some of the differences are artificial in the sense that they are not mutually exclusive. Finally, not every basis for this controversy is linked to the issue of broad versus narrow definitions.
As shown in the first two rows of Table 2.3, the narrow definition restricts violence to the act of assault, regardless of injury, whereas the broad definition defines violence to include multiple modes of maltreatment and the resulting injury. One reason each of these definitions is unacceptable to those who adhere to the other is that more than scientific issues are at stake. Each definition also reflects an underlying moral agenda and professional role as indicated in row F. Consequently, to abandon one or the other definition is tantamount to abandoning that agenda and professional role.
As indicated in Row C of Table 2.3 those using a broad definition tend to be service providers and feminist activists and those using a narrow definition tend to be researchers, but there are many researchers who use a broad definition. A broad definition is essential for service providers. It would be ridiculous and unethical if service providers such, as shelters, batterer treatment programs, or marital therapists, restricted their focus to physical assaults and ignored psychological assaults, sexual coercion, subjugation, and other forms of degradation. On the other hand, researchers tend to focus on investigating one specific type of maltreatment, such as physical assaults, because each type is complex and difficult to investigate. Much can only be learned by a concentrated research focus. 1 believe that most of those who focus on just one form of maltreatment also recognize the need to take into consideration multiple modes of maltreatment, even though they themselves do not conduct that type of research.
The difference in emphasis on injury reflects the different needs of service providers and researchers. For a service provider, it is essential to
38 NATURE OF VIOLENCE
TABLE 2.3. Variables Underlying Broad Versus Narrow Definitions of Violence.
Broad Definition
Narrow Definition
A. Coverage
All types of maltreatment Only physical assault
B. Role of Injury in An inherent part of the
Concept of Violence concept
C. Occupation/Role
Service providers/Feminist activists
One of many possible consequences to be investigated
Academi c s/Researc hers
D. Statistics Favored to police and crime survey Describe Nature of because they show more the Problem women victims and
suggest cause is patriarchy
Family conflict data because they show ubiquity of the problem and suggest multiple causes
E. Statistics Favored to Family conflict because rates Family conflict because rates Mobilize Resources are many times greater are many times greater
F. Pri mary (B u t No t Exclusive) Moral Concern
End oppression of women, regardless of the type of oppression
End physical assaults, regardless of the gender of perpetrator or victim
G. Primary Use of Cessation of assaults on "Primary prevention" of
Research Using the women, especially assaults physical violence of all types, Definition experienced as a "real crime" from spanking to murder
know if the assault resulted in injury because different steps are needed to deal with cases involving injury. For a researcher who is investigating such things as the type of family or type of society in which partner assaults are most likely to occur, injury may not be a crucial issue because it can be assumed that injury occurs in a certain proportion of cases. Moreover, for some purposes it is necessary to exclude injury as a criterion. One of these is research that seeks to estimate the prevalence of domestic assaults. If injury is one of the criteria, it restricts the data to more serious assaults and, as we have seen, the overall prevalence rate is vastly underestimated. Thus, the widely cited figure from the National Family Violence Survey of 1.8 million women severely assaulted each year becomes only 188,000 when the criteria for a severe assault includes injury (Straus, 1990b; Straus, 1991; Straus, 1997). Of course, this is a false dichotomy. As indicated in a previous section, both figures are needed. Feminist activists, for example use both figures (see rows D and E of Table 2.3). They have made extensive use of the 1.8 million figure (often presented as a woman is battered every 15 seconds) to mobilize
Domestic Violence by Women
39
resources. At the same time they also use police, crime survey, and emergency room statistics to show that there are many more women victims (in the sense of injured or fearing injury) than male victims.
Row F of Table 2.3 was included to suggest that, underlying the differences just discussed is a deep seated difference in moral agenda. Those who use a broad definition tend to be primarily concerned with the well being of women. They are, of course, also concerned with physical assaults regardless of who is the victim, but their primary concern is ending maltreatment of women. Moreover, as is to be expected, they are hostile to research that might be used by critics of feminism, and this includes research on assaults by women. On the other hand, those defining violence as a physical assault, tend to place ending physical violence at the top of their agenda, regardless of whether the offender is a man, woman, or child. Of the two evils, physical violence and the oppression of women, physical violence tends to take priority, even though (as in my case) they are also concerned with ending all types of gender inequality and maltreatment.
The last row of Table 2.3 brings us back to the idea that research using a broad definition and emphasizing injury may be most useful for informing programs designed to treat offenders or help victims of repeated severe assault. By contrast, research focusing on the act of assault, most of which does not involve injury but does involve millions of couples, may be most useful for informing programs of "primary prevention," i.e., steps that will prevent physical assaults from ever happening.
It is also important to realize that some of the participants in this controversy do not realize (or deny) that moral agendas and professional roles are involved. For example, Patricia Tjaden (personal communication, March 11, 1998.) wrote, "1 certainly hope that you are not suggesting that I or my colleagues are motivated by any type of moral agenda when we developed and implemented the NVAW Survey." In fact, a moral agenda is evident from the title of her study, the "National Violence Against Women Survey" and from the fact that the original plan was to interview only women and to ask only about their victimization, omitting violence perpetrated by women. Perhaps Tjaden meant that this moral agenda was specified by the sponsoring agencies. I hope she did not mean that because research to provide a deeper understanding of violence against women is a worthy moral goal, it is necessarily unscientific. My own insistence that we also study assaults by women is driven by a different, but I think also worthy, moral agenda—-that of ending violence, regardless of who is the perpetrator.
WILL THE CONTROVERSY END?
The analysis in the preceding section suggests that neither side is motivated to understand the other. Rather, each seeks to impose its perspective because they believe the preferred definition is vital to advancing their moral aeenda and
40
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
professional objectives. In my opinion, that will continue. Moreover, society would lose if either side gives up their perspective because society benefits from the moral agenda and professional contribution of both sides. I for one do not intend to give up attempting to advance the "no violence by anyone" moral agenda that has informed my research on domestic assaults and spanking children for 30 years (Straus, 1994).
I believe humanity needs research inspired by the moral agenda and perspective of those who focus on the oppression of women, regardless of whether the oppression is physical, sexual, psychological or economic; and also research inspired by the moral agenda of those who focus on physical assault, regardless of whether the assault is by a man, woman or child. I even dare to hope that the controversy will be resolved by recognizing the need for both perspectives, and that this will bring an end to attempts to discredit those whose agenda and professional contribution requires a different approach and different perspective.
REFERENCES
Bachman, R., & Saltzman, L. E. (1995). Violence against women: Estimates from the
redesigned survey (BJS Publication No. 154-348). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics. Black, D. (1983). Crime as social control. American Sociological Review, 48, 34-45. Bland, R., & Orn, H. (1986). Family violence and psychiatric disorder. Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry, 31(2), 129-137. Brookoff, D. (1997). Drugs, alcohol, and domestic violence in Memphis (National
Institute of Justice Research Preview No.). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Justice. Buzawa, E. S., & Buzawa, C. G. (Eds.). (1996). Do arrests and restraining orders work?
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Calvert, R. (1974). Criminal and civil liability in husband-wife assaults. In S. K.
Steinmetz & M. A. Straus (Eds.), Violence in the Family (chap. 9). New York: Harper
and Row. Carrado, M., George, M. J., Loxam, E., Jones, L., & Templar, D. (1996). Aggression in
British heterosexual relationships: A descriptive analysis. Aggressive Behavior, 22,
401-415. Cook, P. W. (1997). Abused men: The hidden side of domestic violence. Westport, CT:
Praeger. Cowen, E. (1978). Demystifying primary prevention. In D. G. Forgays (Ed.), Primary
prevention of psychopathology (chap. 2). Hanover, NH: University of New England
Press. Craven, D. (1996). Female Victims of Violent Crime (Bureau of Justice Statistics Selected
Findings No. NCJ-162602). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of
Justice Programs.
Domestic Violence by Women
41
Demaris, A. (1992). Male versus female initiation of aggression: The case of courtship violence. In E. C. Viano (Ed.), Intimate violence: Interdisciplinary perspectives
(pp. 111-120). Bristol, PA: Taylor & Francis. Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1979). Violence against wives: A case against the
patriarch. New York: Free Press. Federal Bureau of Investigation (1995). Crime in the United States, 1993: Uniform
crime reports. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice. Feld, S. L., & Straus, M. A. (1989). Escalation and desistance of wife assault in marriage.
Criminology, 27(1), 141-161. Felson, R. B. (1997). Routine activities and involvement in violence as actor, witness,
or target. Violence and Victims, 12(3), 209-221. Ferraro, K. J. (1989). Policing woman battering. Social Problems, 36(1), 61-74. Ferraro, K. J„ & Johnson, J. M. (1983). How women experience battering: The process
of victimization. Social Problems, 30(3), 325-339. Fiebert, M. S. (1997). Annotated bibliography: References examining assaults by
women on their spouses/partners. In B. M. Dank & R. Refinette (Eds.), Sexual
harassment & sexual consent (Vol. 3, pp. 273-286). New Brunswick: Transaction
Publishers. Fiebert, M. S., & Gonzalez, D. M. (1997). College women who initiate assaults on their
male partners and the reasons offered for such behavior. Psychological Reports, 80,
583-590. Garfinkel, H. (1964). Studies of the routine grounds of everyday activities. Social
Problems, 11 (Winter), 225-250. Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1979). Determinants of violence in the family: Towards
a theoretical integration. In W. R. Burr, R. Hill, F. I. Nye, & I. L. Reiss (Eds.),
Contemporary theories about the family (Vol. 1, chap, 21), New York: Free Press. Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A, (1988). Intimate violence. New York: Simon & Schuster. Giiligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Grandin, E., & Lupri, E. (1997). Intimate violence in Canada and the United States: A
cross-national comparison. Journal of Family Violence, 12(4), 417-443. Gryl, F. E., Stith, S. M, & Bird, G. W. (1991). Close dating relationships among college
students: Differences by use of violence and by gender. Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 8, 243-264. Johnson, M. P. (1995). Patriarchal terrorism and common couple violence: Two forms
of violence against women. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57(May), 283-294. Kaufman Kantor, G., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Response of victims and the police to
assaults on wives. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in
American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families (pp.
473-487). New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers. Kennedy, L. W., & Sacco, V. F. (1998). Crime victims in context. Los Angeles, CA:
Roxbury Publishing. Langan, P. A., & Innes, C, A. (1986). Preventing domestic violence against women
(Special Report No. NCJ 102037). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice,
Bureau of Justice Statistics. Luckenbill, D. F. (1977). Criminal homicide as a situated transaction. Social Problems,
25(2), 176-186.
42
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence. (1969). Report of the Media Task Force. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.
O'Leary, K. D., Barling, J., Arias, I., Rosenbaum, A., Malone, J., & Tyree, A. (1989). Prevalence and stability of physical aggression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57(2), 263-268.
Orne, M. (1962). Amount of experience in experiments as a determinant of performance in later experiments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 776-783.
Peterson, K. S. (1997, Nov. 18). Partners unequal in abuse: Study, women's risk is greater. [ASA Today.
Ptacek, J. (1988). Why do men batter their wives? In K. Yllo & M. Bograd (Eds.), Feminist perspectives on wife abuse (pp. 133-157). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Scanzoni, J. (1978). Sex roles, women's work, and marital conflict. Lexington, MA: Lexington.
Sorenson, S. B., & Teiles, C. A. (1991). Self-reports of spousal violence in a Mexican-American and non-Hispanic white population. Violence and Victims, 6(1), 3-15.
Steinmetz, S. K. (1978). The battered husband syndrome. Victimology, 2, 499-509.
Stets, J. £., & Straus, M. A. (1990). Gender differences in reporting of marital violence and its medical and psychological consequences. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families (pp. 151-165). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Straus, M. A. (1976). Sexual inequality, cultural norms, and wife-beating. In E. C. Viano (Ed.), Victims and society (pp. 543-559). Washington, DC: Visage Press.
Straus, M. A. (1977). A sociological perspective on the prevention and treatment of wifebeating. In M. Roy (Ed.), Battered women. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Straus, M. A. (1979), Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CTS) Scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41(1), 75-88.
Straus, M. A. (1990a). The Conflict Tactics Scales and its critics: An evaluation and new data on validity and reliability. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families (pp. 49-73). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publications.
Straus, M. A. (1990b). Injury, frequency, and the representative sample fallacy in measuring wife beating and child abuse. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families (pp. 75-89). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publications.
Straus, M. A. (1990c). The national family violence surveys. In M. A. Straus & R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptions to violence in 8,145 families (pp. 3-16). New Brunswick: Transaction Publications.
Straus, M. A. (1991). Conceptualizaton and measurement of battering: Implications for public policy. In M. Steinman (Ed,), Woman battering: Policy responses (pp. 19-47). Cincinnati: Anderson Publishing.
Straus, M. A. (1992a). Children as witnesses to marital violence: A risk factor for lifelong problems among a nationally representative sample of American men and women. Paper presented at the 23rd Ross roundtable on critical approaches to common pediatric problems. Ohio: Ross Laboratories.
Straus, M. A. (1992b). Sociological research and social policy: The case of family violence. Sociological Forum, 7(2), 211-237.
Domestic Violence by Women
43
Straus, M. A. (1994). Beating the devil out of them: Corporal punishment in American
families. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass/Lexington Books. Straus, M. A. (1997). Physical assaults by women partners: A major social problem. In
M. R. Walsh (Ed.), Women, men and gender: Ongoing debates (pp. 210-221). New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Straus, M. A. (1998). Characteristics of the National Violence Against Women Study
that might explain the low assault rate for both sexes and the even lower rate for
women. Durham, NH: Family Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire. Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1990). Physical violence in American families: Risk
factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Straus, M. A., Gelles, R. J., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in
the American family. New York: Doubleday. Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The revised
Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data.
Journal of Family Issues, 17(3), 283-316. Straus, M. A., & Hotaling, G. T. (Eds.). (1980). The social causes of husband-wife
violence. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Straus, M. A., Kaufman Kantor, G., & Moore, D. W. (1997). Change in cultural norms
approving marital violence: From 1968 to 1994. In G. Kaufman Kantor &. J. L.
Jasinski (Eds.), Out of the darkness: Contemporary perspectives on family violence.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Straus, M. A., & Paschall, M. J. (1998). Corporal punishment by mothers and child's
cognitive development: A longitudinal study. Paper presented at the 14th World
Congress of Sociology, Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Durham, NH: Family Research
Laboratory, University of New Hampshire. Straus, M. A., & Stewart, J. H. (in press). Corporal punishment by American parents:
National data on prevalence, chronicity, severity, and duration, in relation to child,
and family characteristics. Clinical Child and Family Review, 2. Straus, M. A,, Sugarman, D. B., & Giles-Sims, J, (1997). Spanking by parents and
subsequent antisocial behavior of children. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent
Medicine, /JZ(August), 761-767. Straus, M. A., & Sweet, S. (1992). Verbal/symbolic aggression in couples: Incidence
rates and relationships to personal characteristics. Journal of Marriage and the
Family, 54, 346-357. Straus, M. A., & Yodanis, C. L. (1996). Corporal punishment in adolescence and physical
assaults on spouses later in life: What accounts for the link? Journal of Marriage
and the Family, 58(4), 825-841. Sugarman, D. B., & Hotaling, G. T. (1989). Dating violence: Prevalence, context, and
risk markers. In A. A. Pirog-Good & J. E. Stets (Eds.), Violence in dating relationships: Emerging social issues (pp. 3-31). New York: Praeger. Tjaden, P. G., & Thoennes, N. (1997). The prevalence and consequences of intimate partner violence: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey.
Paper presented at the American Society of Criminology 49th Annual Meeting, San
Diego, CA. U.S. Department of Justice (1976). Dictionary of criminal justice data terminology.
Washington, DC: National Criminal Justice Information Service.
44
NATURE OF VIOLENCE
Vissing, Y. M, Straus, M. A., Gelies, R. J„ & Harrop, J. W. (1993). Verbal aggression by parents and psychosocial problems of children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 223-238.
Zimring, F. E., & Hawkins, G. (1997). Crime is not the problem: Lethal violence in America. New York: Oxford University Press.
Zlotnick, C, Kohn, R., Peterson, J., & Pearlstein, T. (1998). Partner physical victimization in a national sample of American families: Relationship to psychological functioning, psychosocial factors, and gender. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13(1), 156-166.
Keine Kommentare:
Kommentar veröffentlichen